In collaboration with Payame Noor University and Iranian Association of Social Psychology

Document Type : Research Paper

Authors

1 PhD Department of Educational Psychology, Islamic Azad University

2 Assistant Professor of Psychology, Kerman University of Medical Sciences

3 Assistant Professor of Psychology, Islamic Azad University, Kerman Branch

Abstract

Introduction: The purpose of the present study was to study the association between the response inhibition executive functions of response inhibition and working memory and the development of the mind theory in primary school children. Methods: This research method was correlational. The population in this study consisted of all elementary school students in Mashhad in the academic year 1396-95. The sample consisted of 180 students randomly selected and matched by age, gender, and grade. Data collection tools in this study consisted of 38 questions form the Theory of Mind, Morris et al. ( ), Stroop Test and the Wechsler Memory Scale Active. The data collected were analyzed using correlation and linear regression analysis to test the research questions. Results: The results showed a positive and significant relationship between the executive functions of response inhibition, working memory and the development of theory of mind. Also a correlation was found between response inhibition and working memory's ability to predict the level of theory of mind. Furthermore, the results showed a positive relationship between theory of mind and response inhibition and working memory span. Conclusion: In general, executive functions, particularly working memory and response inhibition play decisive roles in anticipating and development of human mental abilities, including the ability of the conceptual mind. In addition, improving these actions led to greater recognition of social human understanding. Therefore, paying attention to learners’ working memory capacity and the effectiveness of learning and teaching materials designed to create the ultimate objective of all of them, or facilitate the development of cognitive functions, display significant effects.

Keywords

- Anderson, D. E.; Vogel, E. K. & et al. (2011). “Precision in visual working memory reaches a stable plateau when individual item limits are exceeded”. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1128-1138. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4125-10.2011
- Carlson, S. M.; Mandell, D. J. & Williams, L. (2004). “Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3”. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105-1122.
- Carlson, S. M.; Claxton, L. J. & Moses, L. J. (2015). “The relation between executive function and theory of mind is more than skin deep”. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 186–197.
- Barkley, R. (2015). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. New York: The Guilford Press.
- Baddeley, A. (2012) “Working memory: theories, models, and controversies”. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1-29.
- Bays, P. M. (2014). “Noise in neural populations accounts for errors in working memory”. Journal of Neuroscien; ce, 34(10), 3632e3645.
- Best, J. R. & Miller, P. H. (2010). “A developmental perspective on executive function”. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. Retrieved from.
- Brandt, S. & Buttelmann, D. (2015, September). Theory of mind and complex syntax. Paper presented at the Developmental Section and Social Section Annual Conference of the British Psychological Society, Manchester, UK.
- Braver, T. S. (2012). “The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual mechanisms framework”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 106–113.
- Brunoni, A. R. & Vanderhasselt, M.-A. (2014). “Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis”. Brain Cogn. 86, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.0.
- Bomyea, J.; amir, n. & lang. A. J. (2012). “The relationship between cognitive control and posttraumatic stress symptoms”. Journal of behavior therapy and expermental psychiatry, 43, 844-848.
- Bock, A. M.; Gallaway, K. C. & Hund, A. M. (2014). “Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding”. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(3).
- Buttelman, D. (2016). “The development of the explicit understanding of own versus others’ beliefs and intentions”. Manuscript in preparation.
- Butterfill, S. A. & Apperly, I. A. (2013). “How to construct a minimal theory of mind”. Mind & Language, 28, 606–637.
- Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. (2013). “Silent films and strange stories: Theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood”. Child Development, 84, 989-1003.
- Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. (2014). “Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis”. Child Development, 85, 1777–1794.
- Diamond, A. (2013). “Executive functions”. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
- de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. (2014). “The role of language in theory of mind development”. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(4), 313–328. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000037
- Howard Gola, A. A. (2012). “Mental verb input for promoting children’s theory of mind: A training study”. Cognitive Development, 27, 64–7.
- Hughes, C. & Ensor, R. (2007). “Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4”. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447-1459.
- Hughes, C. & Devine, R. T. (2015). A social perspective on theory of mind. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Socioemotional processes (7th ed. In & M. E. Lamb (Eds.). Handbook of child psychology (7th ed.) (Vol. 3, pp. 564–609). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
- Hughes, C. & Ensor, R. (2011). “Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age”. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 663–676
- Lewis, C. (2008). “Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding”. Child Development, 79, 80-99.
- Lewis, C.; Koyasu, M.; Oh, S.; Ogawa, A.; Short, B. & Huang, Z. (2009). “Culture, executive function, and social understanding”. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 123, 69–85.
- Robinson, S.; Goddard, L.; Dritschel, B.; Wisley, M. & Howlin, P. (2009). “Executive function in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders”. Brain and Cognition 71, 362-368.
- Ruffman, T. (2014). “To belief or not belief: Children’s theory of mind”. Developmental Review,  34, 265–293.
- Perner, J. & Roessler, J. (2012). “From Infants’ to Children’s Appreciation of Belief”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(10), 519–525.
- Pellicano, E. (2007). “Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy”. Developmental Psychology, 43, 974-990. 
- Mary, A.; Slama, H.; Mousty, P. et al. “Executive and attentional contributions to Theory of Mind defiit in attention defiit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Child”. Neuropsychol 22(3), 345-365 (2016).
- Miranda, A.; Presentación, M. J.; Siegenthaler, R. & Jara, P. (2013). “Effects of a psychosocial intervention on the executive functioning in children with ADHD”. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(4), 363- 376.
- Moses, L. J. & Tahiroglu, D. (2010). Clarifying the relation between executive function and children’s theories of mind. 
- In B. W.; Sokol, U.; Müller, J. I. M.; Carpendale, A. R.; Young, G. & Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 218-233). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Toglia, J. & Berg, C. (2013). “Performance-based measure of executive function: Comparison of community and at-risk youth”. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 67(5), 515- 523.
- Sabbagh, M. A.; Xu, F.; Carlson, S. M.; Moses, L. J. & Lee, K. (2006). “The development of executive functioning and theory of mind”. Psychological Science, 17, 74-81.
- Wong, K. K.; Freeman, D. & Hughes, C. (2014). “Suspicious young minds: Paranoia and mistrust in 8- to 14-year-olds in the UK and Hong Kong”. British Journal of Psychiatry, 205, 221–229.