- Apperly, I.A.; Warren, F.; Andrews, B.J.; Grant, J. and Todd, S. (2011). “Error patterns in the belief-desire reasoning of 3- to 5-year-olds recur in reaction times from 6 years to adulthood: evidence for developmental continuity in theory of mind”.Child Development,82(5), Pp1691-703.
- Apperly, I.A.; Riggs, K.J.; Simpson, A.; Chiavarino, C. and Samson, D. (2006). “Is belief reasoning automatic?”.Psychological Science, 17(10), Pp841-844.
- Apperly, I.A.; Carroll, D.J.; Samson, D.; Humphreys, G.W.; Qureshi, A. and Moffitt, G. (2010). “Why Are There Limits On Theory Of Mind Use? Evidence from Adults' Ability to Follow Instructions froman Ignorant Speaker”. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63 (6), Pp 1201-1217.
- Back, E. and Apperly, I.A. (2010). “Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription”.Cognition,115(1), Pp 54-70.
- Baillargeon, R.; Scott, R.M. and He, Z. (2010). “False-belief understanding in infants”.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, Pp110-118.
- Butterfill, S. andApperly I.A. (2013). “How to construct a minimal theory of mind”. Mindand Language.28 (5),Pp 606-637.
- Cohen, A.S. and German, T.C. (2009). “Encoding of others' beliefs without overt instruction”. Cognition, 111,Pp 356-363.
- Converse, B.A.; Lin, S.; Keysar, B. andEpley, N. (2008). “In the mood to get over yourself: Mood affects theory-of-mind use”. Emotion, 8, Pp725 730.
- Dumontheil, I.; Küster, O.; Apperly, I. A.; Blakemore, S.J.(2010). “Taking perspective into account in a communicativetask” .NeuroImage, 52, Pp1574-1583.
- Epley, N.; Morewedge, C. and Keysar, B. (2004). “Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, Pp760-768.
- Epley, N. (2004). A Tale of Tuned Decks? Anchoring As Accessibility AndAnchoringAs Adjustment. In D. Koehler and N. Harvey (Eds.), The Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making (Pp. 240-257).Malden, Ma: Blackwell.
- German, T.P. and Hehman, J.A. (2006) “Representational and executive selection resources in “theory of mind”: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age”. Cognition, 101, Pp 129-152.
- Keysar, B.; Barr, D.J.; Balin, J.A. andBrauner, J.S. (2000). “Taking perspective in conversation: the role of mutual knowledge in comprehension”. Psychological Sciences, 11,Pp 32-38.
- Kovács, Á.M.; Téglás, E. And Endress, A.D. (2010). “The social sense: susceptibly to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults”. Science, 330, Pp1830-1834.
- Lin, S.; Keysar, B. andEpley, N. (2010). “Reflexivelymindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, Pp551-556.
- Leslie, A.M.; German, T.P. and Polizzi, P. (2005). Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection. Cognitive Psychology, 50, Pp45-85.
- Leslie, A.M. and Thaiss, L. (1992). “Domain specificity inconceptual development: Neuropsychological evidencefromautism ”. Cognition, 43,Pp 225-251.
- McCleery, J.P.; Surtees, A.D.R.; Graham, K.A.; Richards, J.E. and Apperly, I.A. (2011). “The Neural and Cognitive Time Course of Theory of Mind”.The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(36), Pp12849-12854.
- McKinnon, M.C. andMoscovitch, M. (2007). “Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: Theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored”. Cognition, 102(2), Pp 179-218.
- Newton, A.M. and de Villiers, J.G. (2007). Thinking while talking: Adults fail nonverbal false belief reasoning. PsychologicalScience, 18 (7), Pp574-579.
- Onishi, K.H. and Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do15 - month - old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, Pp255-258.
- Phillips, L.H.; Bull, R.; Allen, R.; Insch, P.; Burr, K. and Ogg, W. (2011). “Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive functions and social cue detection”. Cognition, 120, Pp 236-247.
- Qureshi, A.; Apperly, I.A. and Samson, D. (2010). “Executive function is necessary for perspective-selection, not Level-1 visual perspective -calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults”. Cognition, 117(2), Pp230-236.
- Ramsey, R.; Hansen, P.; Apperly, I. and Samson, D. (2013). “Seeing It My Way or Your Way: Frontoparietal Brain Areas Sustain Viewpoint-independent Perspective Selection Processes”. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(5), pp 670-684.
- Samson, D.; Apperly, I.A.; Braithwaite, J.; Andrews, B. And Bodley Scott (2010). “Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36(5),Pp 1255-1266.
- Surtees, A. andApperly, I.A. (2012). “Egocentrism and automatic perspective-taking in children and adults”.Child Development, 83 (2), Pp452-460.
- Shelton, A. and Mcnamara, T.P. (1997). “Multiple Views ofSpatial Memory”. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, Pp 102-106.
- Shelton, A.L. and Mcnamara, T.P. (2001). “Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory”. Cognitive Psychology, 43, pp 274-310.
- Surtees, A.; Butterfill, S. andApperly, I.A. (2012). “Cognitive features of Level-2 Perspective-taking in Children and Adults”. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 30(1), Pp 75-86.